The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model

Detalhes bibliográficos
Autor(a) principal: Nunes,Maria Vania Silva
Data de Publicação: 2009
Outros Autores: Castro-Caldas,Alexandre, Del Rio,Dolores, Maestú,Fernado, Ortiz,Tomás
Tipo de documento: Artigo
Idioma: eng
Título da fonte: Dementia & Neuropsychologia
Texto Completo: http://old.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1980-57642009000300222
Resumo: Abstract The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. Objectives: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. Methods: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. Results: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. Conclusions: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task.
id ANCC-1_76d0fc954976ed5f429b100da50f6c8e
oai_identifier_str oai:scielo:S1980-57642009000300222
network_acronym_str ANCC-1
network_name_str Dementia & Neuropsychologia
repository_id_str
spelling The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD modelilliteracymagnetoencephalographycognitive reservebrain asymmetrylanguageHAROLD model.Abstract The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. Objectives: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. Methods: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. Results: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. Conclusions: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task.Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento2009-09-01info:eu-repo/semantics/articleinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersiontext/htmlhttp://old.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1980-57642009000300222Dementia & Neuropsychologia v.3 n.3 2009reponame:Dementia & Neuropsychologiainstname:Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento (ANCC)instacron:ANCC10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessNunes,Maria Vania SilvaCastro-Caldas,AlexandreDel Rio,DoloresMaestú,FernadoOrtiz,Tomáseng2016-07-29T00:00:00Zoai:scielo:S1980-57642009000300222Revistahttp://www.demneuropsy.com.br/ONGhttps://old.scielo.br/oai/scielo-oai.php||demneuropsy@uol.com.br1980-57641980-5764opendoar:2016-07-29T00:00Dementia & Neuropsychologia - Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento (ANCC)false
dc.title.none.fl_str_mv The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
spellingShingle The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
Nunes,Maria Vania Silva
illiteracy
magnetoencephalography
cognitive reserve
brain asymmetry
language
HAROLD model.
title_short The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_full The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_fullStr The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_full_unstemmed The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_sort The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
author Nunes,Maria Vania Silva
author_facet Nunes,Maria Vania Silva
Castro-Caldas,Alexandre
Del Rio,Dolores
Maestú,Fernado
Ortiz,Tomás
author_role author
author2 Castro-Caldas,Alexandre
Del Rio,Dolores
Maestú,Fernado
Ortiz,Tomás
author2_role author
author
author
author
dc.contributor.author.fl_str_mv Nunes,Maria Vania Silva
Castro-Caldas,Alexandre
Del Rio,Dolores
Maestú,Fernado
Ortiz,Tomás
dc.subject.por.fl_str_mv illiteracy
magnetoencephalography
cognitive reserve
brain asymmetry
language
HAROLD model.
topic illiteracy
magnetoencephalography
cognitive reserve
brain asymmetry
language
HAROLD model.
description Abstract The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. Objectives: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. Methods: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. Results: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. Conclusions: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task.
publishDate 2009
dc.date.none.fl_str_mv 2009-09-01
dc.type.driver.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type.status.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
format article
status_str publishedVersion
dc.identifier.uri.fl_str_mv http://old.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1980-57642009000300222
url http://old.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1980-57642009000300222
dc.language.iso.fl_str_mv eng
language eng
dc.relation.none.fl_str_mv 10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008
dc.rights.driver.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
eu_rights_str_mv openAccess
dc.format.none.fl_str_mv text/html
dc.publisher.none.fl_str_mv Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento
publisher.none.fl_str_mv Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento
dc.source.none.fl_str_mv Dementia & Neuropsychologia v.3 n.3 2009
reponame:Dementia & Neuropsychologia
instname:Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento (ANCC)
instacron:ANCC
instname_str Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento (ANCC)
instacron_str ANCC
institution ANCC
reponame_str Dementia & Neuropsychologia
collection Dementia & Neuropsychologia
repository.name.fl_str_mv Dementia & Neuropsychologia - Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento (ANCC)
repository.mail.fl_str_mv ||demneuropsy@uol.com.br
_version_ 1754212929650032640