A note on conversational interruptions

Detalhes bibliográficos
Autor(a) principal: Deyes, Anthony F.
Data de Publicação: 1984
Tipo de documento: Artigo
Idioma: por
Título da fonte: Ilha do Desterro
Texto Completo: https://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/article/view/10878
Resumo: In their seminal article, Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson (1974) examine procedures for turn-taking in conversation. Sometimes, they note, a s peaker will select who has the next turn, but more frequently a "self-select" system operates, whereby the participants in a conversation themselves determine when they wish to speak. But how, ask Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson, do the interlocutors secure a turn in the ongoing flow of another speaker's utterance? To answer this question the authors introduce the notion of "transition relevance place", that is, a point in the turn-holder's utterance where another speaker is most likely to take up a turn. Clearly the most obvious transition relevance place occurs at the end of an utterance sentence, where a pause may be made. However, clause or thersyntactic boundaries also offer opportunities for other speakers to interrupt. As Sacks et al point out, if conversational participants do not take up a turn at a transition relevance place the turn holder will normally continue.
id UFSC-9_7ca380b7fb64809e517216cf3cf8effa
oai_identifier_str oai:periodicos.ufsc.br:article/10878
network_acronym_str UFSC-9
network_name_str Ilha do Desterro
repository_id_str
spelling A note on conversational interruptionsA note on conversational interruptionsIn their seminal article, Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson (1974) examine procedures for turn-taking in conversation. Sometimes, they note, a s peaker will select who has the next turn, but more frequently a "self-select" system operates, whereby the participants in a conversation themselves determine when they wish to speak. But how, ask Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson, do the interlocutors secure a turn in the ongoing flow of another speaker's utterance? To answer this question the authors introduce the notion of "transition relevance place", that is, a point in the turn-holder's utterance where another speaker is most likely to take up a turn. Clearly the most obvious transition relevance place occurs at the end of an utterance sentence, where a pause may be made. However, clause or thersyntactic boundaries also offer opportunities for other speakers to interrupt. As Sacks et al point out, if conversational participants do not take up a turn at a transition relevance place the turn holder will normally continue.In their seminal article, Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson (1974) examine procedures for turn-taking in conversation. Sometimes, they note, a s peaker will select who has the next turn, but more frequently a "self-select" system operates, whereby the participants in a conversation themselves determine when they wish to speak. But how, ask Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson, do the interlocutors secure a turn in the ongoing flow of another speaker's utterance? To answer this question the authors introduce the notion of "transition relevance place", that is, a point in the turn-holder's utterance where another speaker is most likely to take up a turn. Clearly the most obvious transition relevance place occurs at the end of an utterance sentence, where a pause may be made. However, clause or thersyntactic boundaries also offer opportunities for other speakers to interrupt. As Sacks et al point out, if conversational participants do not take up a turn at a transition relevance place the turn holder will normally continue.UFSC1984-01-01info:eu-repo/semantics/articleinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersionapplication/pdfhttps://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/article/view/10878Ilha do Desterro A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies; Vol. 5 No. 11 (1984); 075-082Ilha do Desterro A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies; v. 5 n. 11 (1984); 075-0822175-80260101-4846reponame:Ilha do Desterroinstname:Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina (UFSC)instacron:UFSCporhttps://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/article/view/10878/10359Copyright (c) 1984 Anthony F. Deyeshttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessDeyes, Anthony F.2022-12-06T12:43:22Zoai:periodicos.ufsc.br:article/10878Revistahttp://www.periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterroPUBhttps://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/oaiilha@cce.ufsc.br||corseuil@cce.ufsc.br||ilhadodesterro@gmail.com2175-80260101-4846opendoar:2022-12-06T12:43:22Ilha do Desterro - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina (UFSC)false
dc.title.none.fl_str_mv A note on conversational interruptions
A note on conversational interruptions
title A note on conversational interruptions
spellingShingle A note on conversational interruptions
Deyes, Anthony F.
title_short A note on conversational interruptions
title_full A note on conversational interruptions
title_fullStr A note on conversational interruptions
title_full_unstemmed A note on conversational interruptions
title_sort A note on conversational interruptions
author Deyes, Anthony F.
author_facet Deyes, Anthony F.
author_role author
dc.contributor.author.fl_str_mv Deyes, Anthony F.
description In their seminal article, Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson (1974) examine procedures for turn-taking in conversation. Sometimes, they note, a s peaker will select who has the next turn, but more frequently a "self-select" system operates, whereby the participants in a conversation themselves determine when they wish to speak. But how, ask Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson, do the interlocutors secure a turn in the ongoing flow of another speaker's utterance? To answer this question the authors introduce the notion of "transition relevance place", that is, a point in the turn-holder's utterance where another speaker is most likely to take up a turn. Clearly the most obvious transition relevance place occurs at the end of an utterance sentence, where a pause may be made. However, clause or thersyntactic boundaries also offer opportunities for other speakers to interrupt. As Sacks et al point out, if conversational participants do not take up a turn at a transition relevance place the turn holder will normally continue.
publishDate 1984
dc.date.none.fl_str_mv 1984-01-01
dc.type.driver.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/article
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
format article
status_str publishedVersion
dc.identifier.uri.fl_str_mv https://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/article/view/10878
url https://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/article/view/10878
dc.language.iso.fl_str_mv por
language por
dc.relation.none.fl_str_mv https://periodicos.ufsc.br/index.php/desterro/article/view/10878/10359
dc.rights.driver.fl_str_mv Copyright (c) 1984 Anthony F. Deyes
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
rights_invalid_str_mv Copyright (c) 1984 Anthony F. Deyes
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0
eu_rights_str_mv openAccess
dc.format.none.fl_str_mv application/pdf
dc.publisher.none.fl_str_mv UFSC
publisher.none.fl_str_mv UFSC
dc.source.none.fl_str_mv Ilha do Desterro A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies; Vol. 5 No. 11 (1984); 075-082
Ilha do Desterro A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies; v. 5 n. 11 (1984); 075-082
2175-8026
0101-4846
reponame:Ilha do Desterro
instname:Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina (UFSC)
instacron:UFSC
instname_str Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina (UFSC)
instacron_str UFSC
institution UFSC
reponame_str Ilha do Desterro
collection Ilha do Desterro
repository.name.fl_str_mv Ilha do Desterro - Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina (UFSC)
repository.mail.fl_str_mv ilha@cce.ufsc.br||corseuil@cce.ufsc.br||ilhadodesterro@gmail.com
_version_ 1799875276939722753